18 févr. 2020

Triathlon and Self-Awareness


We work with athletes and patients.  The key to have results is self-awareness; we cannot lie to ourselves if we want results.  An example of what we should not want from athletes is lack of self-awareness as in the case of the highest rank official of the FMTRI which comment highlights lack of self-awareness:
“It is not stupidity to say that Crisanto Grajales could be in the top six in Tokyo.” His premise is based on the fact that 65 competitors started the Tokyo race last year and just finished 46, and Grajales run a second faster than Mislowchuk and Stornes (the first two), a minute behind on the bike; not counting that Mola, Gómez, Luis and others were not there.  And the fact the event’s page said: price money TBA for competing in that race.  Some competitors were there to test the bike course.  That comment gives information about the lack of self-awareness or perhaps he is just “Trumping.” 

Researchers are working on how to measure self-awareness in athletes.  The constructs are not easy to develop but as any science, it does not proof anything, it just probes.  We have known the facts empirically for centuries.
Introduction
Mental toughness (MT) is widely recognised as a fundamental attribute for attaining success in sport.1 Mentally tougher athletes maintain performance levels during adversity; perceive pressure as a challenge and a catalyst for prospering; and maintain emotional, cognitive and behavioural control despite situational stressors.2 Considering the appeal that these cognitive and behavioural signatures have to athletes, MT has become a prominent research area in the sport performance literature.3
Scholars' primary interest in MT is based on the capacity to acquire MT attributes through sport and non-sport developmental influences and experiences4, as well as through psychological interventions5. However, determining the MT dimensions that may be taught and the most effective approaches to develop them requires resolutions to the current conceptual and operational disparities that exist. Some researchers contend that MT is a narrow personality trait that is situationally stable5,6, whereas others suggest MT is state-specific and may fluctuate depending on the situation7,8. In addition to MT manifestation distinctions, these conceptualisations differ in the extent to which MT may be developed. However, in support of the mutual inclusivity of these perspectives, Gucciardi et al.3 reported that a combination of intraindividual (i.e. within person) and interindividual (i.e. between person) differences may be attributed to the variability of MT. Accordingly, an athlete may display enduring patterns of MT across similar situations, but varied levels of MT across dissimilar situations.
Although the multidimensionality of MT has generally been supported9, the type and quantity of constituents comprising MT remains unclear10. In addition to dimensional discrepancies between sport types,11 within-sport MT differences have been found. For instance, Coulter et al.12 reported that risk-taking is an integral MT component in soccer, whereas Thelwell et al.13 indicated that MT in a soccer player involved affecting one's opponents. The characterisation of MT variations are reflected in the range of instruments that often diverge in the types of MT that are measured. To illustrate, affective intelligence is included as a subfactor on the Cricket Mental Toughness Inventory14, but is not contained within the Australian Football Mental Toughness Inventory15.
Although unequivocally determining the components that constitute MT is necessary, there are several components that are repeatedly referred to in the literature.16 These components include confidence or self-belief; emotional and cognitive control; accepting, persevering and thriving through challenges; and commitment and determination.2,17 Accordingly, MT refers to a collection of personal resources (inherent and developed) associated with athletes' pursuit of optimal athletic performance levels, irrespective of positive and negative situational demands.18,19
In the extant literature, considerable attention has been devoted towards examining the characteristics associated with MT. Commonly identified correlates of MT include effective coping, the use of self-talk, relaxation strategies and mental imagery.20-22 Mentally tougher athletes have greater flow experiences (concentration, autotelism)23, perceive stressors as less intense24, and utilise performance- and mastery-approach achievement goals25. Collectively, MT is related to a number of positive psychological characteristics. However, self-awareness, also referred to as psychological self-mindedness, is one concept that has received limited quantitative MT research attention. Self-awareness represents the capacity to attend to, recognise and examine one's thoughts, physiological sensations, emotions and behavioural reactions, either as they occur or retrospectively.26,27
Although the self-awareness process is multifaceted and associated with an array of corollaries and self-directed attention areas26, common conceptualisations encompass two primary components: engagement in self-reflection and the attainment of self-insight28-30. Self-reflection involves emotional, cognitive and behavioural self-introspection, whereas self-insight refers to clarifying and obtaining a deeper understanding of such experiences.29
Even though self-reflective activities may not automatically result in self-insight31, self-awareness represents an important process for identifying and replacing maladaptive responses as well as establishing progress towards achieving positive psychobehavioural changes28,32.
In sport, awareness of one's emotions has been linked to superior performance.33 In particular, maintaining peak performance levels is at least partly dependent on the ability to recognise negative emotions and cognitions and effectively control or avoid the detrimental effects of such experiences.34 With research supporting the emotional and cognitive control of mentally tough athletes35, along with the understanding that MT is associated with positive performance outcomes3, self-awareness attributes may be relevant to athletes' MT.
Recent qualitative research has posited the relevance of several forms of self-awareness (e.g. emotional and cognitive) in relation to MT. Bull et al.25, for instance, qualitatively established thinking clearly (awareness, focus and control of thoughts) as an essential component of MT in elite cricket. Slack et al.36 extended this finding to denote cognitive awareness of own emotions as indicative of mentally tough English Premier League football referees. There is also evidence to suggest that self-awareness promotes or facilitates heightened levels of MT37 - a finding that supports early heuristic MT perspectives38.
Taken together, these findings provide preliminary support for the applicability of self-awareness characteristics to the MT of athletes. However, prior MT studies have not specified what embodies self-awareness, and, despite recent qualitative findings, there is a dearth of knowledge about the role of emotional, cognitive and behavioural self-awareness in relation to MT. Therefore, the purpose of the current study was to explore the relationships between MT and self-awareness components (i.e. self-reflection and self-insight) in competitive tennis players. It was hypothesised that MT and each of its subcomponents would be significantly predicted by both (1) self-reflection and (2) self-insight.
The finding that self-insight was the single significant predictor of global MT suggests the phase is particularly important to athletes' MT. However, given that attaining insight requires introspection and evaluation of the self,31 self-reflection is a necessary part of the self-awareness process. Considering the markedly larger effect size between MT and self-insight, as compared to self-reflection, mentally tougher athletes appear to be better at progressing from self-reflective activities to achieve higher levels of self-insight. With prior studies reporting that self-awareness promotes the development of MT,37 attaining maximal MT benefits might require athletes to engage in and proceed beyond mere self-introspection toward generating a profounder level of psychobehavioural clarity and understanding.
Conclusion
The findings in this study support the positive association between MT and self-awareness in competitive tennis players. Most notably, the strongest predictor of MT and its subcomponents was self-insight. Notwithstanding the necessity of self-reflection in the process toward obtaining insight, the latter appears to be particularly important when considering MT and its development among athletes. Research identifying the contextual demands and situation-based use of self-awareness among mentally tough athletes is warranted, along with whether self-reflection and insight may be used to develop MT through interventions.

We work on education with the purpose of increasing self-awareness to have better triathletes.